[...] Moriguchi et al. (2005) measured task-related brain activity while Japanese and American participants observed fearful facial expressions. The Japanese participants demonstrated more task-related activity in the right inferior frontal gyrus, pre-motor cortex, and left insula, whereas the American participants demonstrated more task-related activity in the posterior cingulate, supplementary motor cortex, and amygdala. The authors concluded that “Americans respond to fearful faces in a more direct, emotional way, whereas Japanese do not attach an emotional valence to the faces.”
Hugdahl et al. (2006) measured task-related activity while male and female participants were performing equally well on a mental rotation task (using Shepard & Metzler, 1971, stimuli). The male participants demonstrated more task-related activity in parietal areas, whereas the female participants demonstrated more task-related activity in inferior frontal regions. The authors concluded that “males may be biased toward a coordinate processing approach, and females biased toward a serial, categorical processing approach.”
When mothers viewed photos of their own children, task-related activity in the amygdala and insula was interpreted as “reflecting the intense attachment, vigilant protectiveness, and empathy that characterize normal maternal attachment” (Leibenluft et al., 2004). When boyfriends listened to sentences such as “my girlfriend gave a gorgeous birthday present to her ex-boyfriend,” task-related activity in the amygdala and insula was interpreted as identifying the “brain regions involved in sexual/aggressive behavior” (Takahashi et al., 2006).
When college-aged participants successfully retrieved episodic memories, task-related activity in frontal regions was considered a sign of higher-order reasoning; when participants of the modal age of APS members successfully retrieved episodic memories, task-related activity in frontal regions was considered “compensation” (Aine et al., 2006). From reading such brain-imaging studies, one would think that amateur psychoanalysts, rather than card-carrying neuroscientists, were driving the interpretations. For my own ride, I’d like to have social psychologists in the passenger seat, bringing along their decades of research on the unfortunate effects of stereotyping, bias, and prejudice.
For example, group comparisons in many functional brain-imaging studies illustrate quite vividly Miller et al.’s (1991) thesis, drawn from behavioral and social psychological research: Explanations for differences between prototypic and nonprototypic groups use the former to explain the latter. Thus, in brain-imaging studies, nonprototypic groups are described as demonstrating more versus less task-related activity — where more and less are always in reference to the prototypic group.
Rather than being appreciated as functional adaptations — as when blind participants activate visual cortex while reading braille (Burton et al., 2002) and Deaf participants activate auditory cortex while perceiving motion (Fine et al., 2005) — levels of activation are often judged in Goldilocks fashion (too much, too little, just right), and regions of activation are often interpreted as if they were Zodiac signs.
As we explore the neural underpinnings of tasks as diverse as grocery shopping (Braeutigam et al., 2004) and tickling (Blakemore et al., 2000), and we recruit participants who are also diverse, let’s consider the possibility of diversity in neural structure and function. And let’s do so without biased inferences or stereotypic interpretations.
[APS President Morton Ann Gernsbacher, University of Wisconsin-Madison]
http://www.psychologicalscience.org/observer/getArticle.cfm?id=2134